Foraging Behavior of Birds at Morning and Evening Times: A Preliminary Study
Abstract
Abstract Views: 773Birds exhibit a wide variety of behaviors including foraging behavior which could vary both inter- and intra-specifically. The current study was aimed to investigate different aspects of the foraging behavior of birds in different fields situated at the Quaid-e-Azam Campus, University of the Punjab, Lahore. The said campus covers an area of about 721 ha and has a stable ecosystem which supports a diverse range of birds. Three fields were selected for study purposes. The first field (F1) was characterized by dense vegetation. The second field (F2) contained relatively less vegetation and the third field (F3) was situated near the premises containing the hostel and the shopping center. Observations were recorded during December 2019 for a week at morning (0800-0900 h) and evening (1500-1600 h) times. The observations were based on the assumptions that an abundant availability of food will increase the stay time and vice versa. Moreover, number of pecks by birds also correlate with stay time. A total of 10, 8 and 5 bird species were observed in the above mentioned three fields, that is, F1, F2 and F3, respectively. Different patterns of bird frequency, pecking rate and search time were observed in all fields at morning and evening times which could be associated with the nature of the microhabitat, vegetation, predator risk, food availability, group size and various environmental factors such as temperature. Spearman’s correlation revealed a significant (p < 0.01) association between search time and number of pecks of birds in all three fields.
Copyright(c) The Authors
Downloads
References
. Okore OOw, Amadi C. Bird species of Mouau with special emphasis on foraging behavior of the northern grey-headed sparrow (Passer griseus). Anim Res Int. 2016; 13(1): 2338-2344.
. Mehmood S, Khan BN, Raza H, et al. Assessment of seasonal distribution and threats to avian fauna of Lahore Safari Zoo. Pak J Zool. 2018; 50(2): 533-538.
. Liker A, Barta Z. The effects of dominance on social foraging tactic use in house sparrows. Behaviour. 2002: 1061-1076.
. Giraldeau L-A, Caraco T. Social foraging theory. Vol. 73. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 2018.
. Cueto VR, De Casenave JL. Foraging behavior and microhabitat use of birds inhabiting coastal woodlands in eastcentral Argentina. Wilson Bull. 2002: 342-348.
. Beck MJ, George TL. Song post and foraging site characteristics of breeding varied thrushes in Northwestern California. The Condor. 2000; 102(1): 93-103.
. Bednekoff PA, Lima SL. Why are scanning patterns so variable? An overlooked question in the study of anti-predator vigilance. J Avian Biol. 2002; 33(2): 143-149.
. Krause J, Ruxton GD, Ruxton GD, Ruxton IG. Living in groups. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002.
. Ale SB, Brown JS. The contingencies of group size and vigilance. Evol Ecol Res. 2007; 9(8): 1263-1276.
. Whelan CJ. Foliage structure influences foraging of insectivorous forest birds: an experimental study. Ecology. 2001; 82(1): 219-231.
. Boas MA, Clemens JL, Frys EA, Mortzfeldt SA. Effects of predatory threats on songbird foraging behavior. J Ecol Res. 2001; 11: 23-26.
. Lendvai AZ, Barta Z, Liker A, Bókony V. The effect of energy reserves on social foraging: hungry sparrows scrounge more. Proc R Soc, Ser B. 2004; 271(1556): 2467-2472.
. Schulenberg TS, Gerbracht J. Neotropical birds online-a new resource for Caribbean Ornithologists and birders. J Caribb Ornithol. 2010; 22(1): 60.
. Fernández E-J, Gall MD, Dolan T, Tisdale V, Martin GR. The visual fields of two ground-foraging birds, House Finches and House Sparrows, allow for simultaneous foraging and anti-predator vigilance. Ibis. 2008; 150(4): 779-787.
. Arnaiz A-V, Gomez P-P, Ruiz-del-Valle V. Phylogeography of finches and sparrows: animal genetics. New York, NY: Nova Pub; 2009.
. Johnson C, Grant JW, Giraldeau L-A. The effect of handling time on interference among house sparrows foraging at different seed densities. Behaviour. 2001; 138(5): 597-614.
. Grimmett R, Roberts T, Inskipp T. Birds of Pakistan: Helm Field Guides. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2001.
. Mirza ZB, Wasiq H. Field guide to birds of Pakistan. Lahore: WWF; 2012.
. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences. 1992; 1(3): 98-101. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768783
. Sidra S, Ali Z, Chaudhry MN. Avian diversity at new campus of Punjab University in relation to land use change. Pak J Zool. 2013; 45(4): 1069-1082.
. Tanveer A, Shahzad M, Chaudhry A. Avian fauna of Punjab University, Lahore. Punjab Univ J Zool. 2002; 17: 35-51.
. Strong AM, Sherry TW. Habitat-specific effects of food abundance on the condition of ovenbirds wintering in Jamaica. J Anim Ecol. 2000; 69(5): 883-895.
. Fayt P. Insect prey population changes in habitats with declining vs. stable three-toed woodpecker Picoides tridactylus populations. Ornis Fenn. 2003; 80(4): 182-192.
. Díaz L. Influences of forest type and forest structure on bird communities in oak and pine woodlands in Spain. For Ecol Manag. 2006; 223(1-3): 54-65.
. Watson DM, Herring M. Mistletoe as a keystone resource: an experimental test. Proc R Soc, B. 2012; 279(1743): 3853-3860.
. Polo V, Bautista LM. Daily routines of body mass gain in birds: 1. An exponential model. Anim Behav. 2006; 72(3): 503-516.
. Farine DR, Lang SD. The early bird gets the worm: foraging strategies of wild songbirds lead to the early discovery of food sources. Biol Lett. 2013; 9(6): 20130578.
. Kronfeld N-S, Dayan T. Partitioning of time as an ecological resource. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2003; 34(1): 153-181.
. Silva AD, Diez D-M, Kempenaers B. Effects of experimental night lighting on the daily timing of winter foraging in common European songbirds. J Avian Biol. 2017; 48(6): 862-871.
. Bonter DN, Zuckerberg B, Sedgwick CW, Hochachka WM. Daily foraging patterns in free-living birds: exploring the predation–starvation trade-off. Proc R Soc, B. 2013; 280(1760): 20123087.
. Gentle LK, Gosler AG. Fat reserves and perceived predation risk in the great tit, Parus major. Proc R Soc, B. 2001; 268(1466): 487-491.
. Jones KA, Krebs JR, Whittingham MJ. Interaction between seed crypsis and habitat structure influence patch choice in a granivorous bird, the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. J Avian Biol. 2006; 37(5): 413-418.
. Van Oers K, Drent PJ, De Goede P, Van Noordwijk AJ. Realized heritability and repeatability of risk-taking behaviour in relation to avian personalities. Proc R Soc, B. 2004; 271(1534): 65-73.
. Tvardíková K, Fuchs R. Do birds behave according to dynamic risk assessment theory? a feeder experiment. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011; 65(4): 727-733.
. McNamara JM, Houston AI. Risk-sensitive foraging: a review of the theory. Bull Math Biol. 1992; 54(2-3): 355-378.
. Baker DJ, Stillman RA, Bullock JM. The effect of habitat complexity on the functional response of a seed-eating passerine. Ibis. 2009; 151(3): 547-558.
. Wolf LL, Hainsworth FR. Temporal patterning of feeding by hummingbirds. Anim Behav. 1977; 25: 976-89.
Copyright (c) 2020 Muhammad Abrar Yousaf, Misbah Noreen, Muhammad Faisal Maqbool, Amina Irfan, Ishfaq Ahmad, Memoona
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
BSR follows an open-access publishing policy and full text of all published articles is available free, immediately upon publication of an issue. The journal’s contents are published and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC-BY 4.0) license. Thus, the work submitted to the journal implies that it is original, unpublished work of the authors (neither published previously nor accepted/under consideration for publication elsewhere). On acceptance of a manuscript for publication, a corresponding author on the behalf of all co-authors of the manuscript will sign and submit a completed the Copyright and Author Consent Form.